Giant rat border agents could help put a stop to wildlife poaching

The rodents learned to sniff out elephant ivory and other goods from at-risk species

A rat wearing a red vest holds a small brown ball.

This giant African pouched rat is being trained to signal for illegally smuggled wildlife products. It pulls on a small ball on its collar to alert its handler.

APOPO

A new kind of border-patrol agent could soon sniff out smuggled goods in African ports. They’re rats that wear tiny red vests.

Like dogs, African giant pouched rats have a great sense of smell. Researchers thought the rodents might be able to detect illegal goods that are smuggled across country lines. So trainers taught them to identify items from at-risk species. The animals might be able to help protect those species by finding smuggled ones before they could be sold.

Many tourists visit regions like West Africa to see wildlife. But some species are disappearing. Poachers add to this problem by catching animals for the pet trade. Or they may illegally kill some for their parts. They might hack horns off living rhinos, for instance. Or they might leave dead elephants to rot on a forest floor. Products like rhino horns or elephant tusks are usually taken to other countries to be sold.

Poaching and smuggling are destroying biodiversity, says Isabelle Szott, a behavioral ecologist. “It’s very brutal.”

A rat wearing a tiny red vest stands near a pile of boxes.
This rat learned to identify scents in the lab. Now it’s trying its skills in a situation more like real life.APOPO

Szott used to work in Tanzania, Africa, with a group called APOPO. This group has trained African pouched rats (Cricetomys ansorgei) to put their noses to use before. For example, some of these rats learned to detect dangerous weapons. Some can even sniff out a lung disease called tuberculosis.

People at APOPO thought the rats also had potential at ports, where products enter and leave a country. Besides their incredible sense of smell, these rats are agile and cost little to care for.

Szott’s team taught 11 rat recruits how to tell the difference between odors. The laboratory was a giant box outfitted with 10 small chambers. The researchers placed different samples in each chamber. Some were from animals that get poached. Others were not. The researchers gave the rats food when they kept their nose above the target odor for three seconds. That pose signaled that the rat had found something.

A woman feeds a rat a treat through a giant syringe while touching its head.
When it signals that it has detected a target odor, a rat is rewarded with a flavorful snack.APOPO

The rats learned to signal when they found scales from a pangolin, a threatened small mammal. They also signaled when they sniffed rhino horn, elephant tusk and African blackwood (used in some musical instruments). Eventually, rats who completed these tests could tell the four scents apart from 146 other odors. Szott was part of a team that reported their results October 29 in Frontiers in Conservation Science.

Training the rats to detect scents in the lab is only the beginning, says Szott. Some went on to get lessons in fake warehouses and even in real ports.

When the APOPO rats get too old to be wildlife detectives, she says, they retire. They’ll “just live out their days,” pampered with fruits, veggies and sun-dried fish. 

At the San Diego Zoo, these super-sniffer rats show how they might hunt for smuggled wildlife at ports, in cargo holds and elsewhere.

Sophie Hartley is a Fall 2024 science writing intern at Science News. She holds a bachelor’s degree in Comparative Human Development and Creative Writing from the University of Chicago and a master’s degree in science writing from MIT. Her work has appeared in Science FridayThe Boston Globe MagazineArs Technica, and elsewhere.